Published on in Vol 7, No 11 (2019): November

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/12298, first published .
Effects of a Smartphone-Based Approach-Avoidance Intervention on Chocolate Craving and Consumption: Randomized Controlled Trial

Effects of a Smartphone-Based Approach-Avoidance Intervention on Chocolate Craving and Consumption: Randomized Controlled Trial

Effects of a Smartphone-Based Approach-Avoidance Intervention on Chocolate Craving and Consumption: Randomized Controlled Trial

Original Paper

1Schoen Clinic Roseneck, Prien am Chiemsee, Germany

2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany

3Department of MultiMediaTechnology, Salzburg University of Applied Sciences, Puch, Austria

4Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria

Corresponding Author:

Adrian Meule, Dipl-Psych, PhD

Schoen Clinic Roseneck

Am Roseneck 6

Prien am Chiemsee, 83209

Germany

Phone: 49 8051 68 100 174

Email: ameule@med.lmu.de


Background: Repeatedly pushing high-calorie food stimuli away based on joystick movements has been found to reduce approach biases toward these stimuli. Some studies also found that such avoidance training reduced consumption of high-calorie foods.

Objective: This study aimed to test effects of a smartphone-based approach-avoidance intervention on chocolate craving and consumption, to make such interventions suitable for daily use.

Methods: Within a 10-day period, regular chocolate eaters (n=105, 86% female) performed five sessions during which they continuously avoided (ie, swiped upward) chocolate stimuli (experimental group, n=35), performed five sessions during which they approached and avoided chocolate stimuli equally often (placebo control group, n=35), or did not perform any training sessions (inactive control group, n=35). Training effects were measured during laboratory sessions before and after the intervention period and further continuously through daily ecological momentary assessment.

Results: Self-reported chocolate craving and consumption as well as body fat mass significantly decreased from pre- to postmeasurement across all groups. Ecological momentary assessment reports evidenced no differences in chocolate craving and consumption between intervention days and rest days as a function of the group.

Conclusions: A smartphone-based approach-avoidance training did not affect eating-related and anthropometric measures over and above measurement-based changes in this study. Future controlled studies need to examine whether other techniques of modifying food approach tendencies show an add-on benefit over conventional, monitoring-based intervention effects.

Trial Registration: AsPredicted 8203; https://aspredicted.org/pt9df.pdf.

JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2019;7(11):e12298

doi:10.2196/12298

Keywords



Training individuals to avoid appetitive stimuli has been found to reduce automatic approach tendencies toward these stimuli. For example, repeatedly pushing pictures of alcoholic beverages away on a screen based on joystick movements has been found to reduce approach biases toward alcohol in heavy drinkers [1] and patients with alcohol use disorder [2,3]. Similar results have been obtained using pictures of high-calorie foods in samples of high trait food cravers [4] or individuals with obesity [5-7]. Although effects on actual consumption behaviors is less consistent [8-10], several studies point toward a decrease in craving for and consumption of appetitive substances through approach-avoidance training [11].

While traditional approach-avoidance tasks (AATs) and training are usually performed with joystick movements in front of a computer monitor, methods that make these techniques suitable for daily use are needed. One possibility for this is to implement AATs or training on smartphones. For example, 2 recent studies used a smartphone-based training during which participants were required to swipe pictures away or toward themselves to reduce body dissatisfaction [12] or procrastination [13]. Although these studies reported promising results (ie, changes in behavior because of the approach-avoidance intervention), interpretation was limited by the use of inactive (waitlist) control groups and by combining the training with conventional face-to-face treatment elements.

The aim of this study was, therefore, to evaluate a smartphone-based approach-avoidance training for reducing food craving and consumption in a randomized, fully controlled trial (ie, by comparing active training effects to placebo and no training groups). As chocolate is the most frequently craved food in Western societies [14,15], we restricted our study to chocolate-containing foods, similar to previous studies on approach-avoidance modification [16-18]. Specifically, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 groups: during a 10-day period, they either performed five training sessions during which they continuously avoided pictures of chocolate-containing foods (upward swipes) and approached pictures of neutral objects (downward swipes; experimental group), performed 5 training sessions during which they approached and avoided food and neutral stimuli equally often (placebo control group), or did not perform an approach-avoidance training (inactive control group). All participants completed an AAT and reported their craving for and consumption of chocolate-containing foods before and after the 10-day period. Furthermore, previous studies found short-term effects of approach-avoidance training on food consumption (eg, reduced chocolate muffin consumption in a taste test immediately after an avoidance training session [18]). To capture such short-lived effects, participants reported their craving for and consumption of chocolate-containing foods on each evening during the 10-day period. This allowed us to examine both short-term training effects by comparing chocolate craving and consumption on intervention versus rest days during the 10-day period and long-term training effects by comparing pre- versus posttest values before and after the 10-day period.

We tested the following preregistered hypotheses (Multimedia Appendix 1):

(1) Similar to findings showing that an approach bias modification training decreased approach bias toward high-calorie foods [4], we expected that approach bias toward chocolate-containing foods would decrease from pre- to posttest only in the experimental group but not in the 2 control groups.

(2) Similar to findings showing that self-monitoring of snacking decreases snack food consumption [19], we expected that self-reported chocolate craving and consumption in the past 10 days would decrease from pre- to posttest in all 3 groups, as all participants were confronted with their chocolate consumption behavior during the study. However, because of craving- and consumption-reducing effects of approach-avoidance training found in previous studies [4,18], we expected that these decreases would be larger in the experimental group than in the placebo control group and the inactive control group.

(3) Performing reaction time tasks involving palatable food pictures usually increases food craving from immediately before to immediately after the task [20,21]. Therefore, we expected that performing a chocolate-related AAT would induce chocolate craving, that is, current chocolate craving would be increased immediately after having performed the task compared with before. At pretest, we expected that these chocolate craving increases during the task would be similar in all 3 groups. As previous findings indicate that approach-avoidance training can decrease such food cue–induced craving [4], we expected that task-induced chocolate craving would be attenuated at posttest in the experimental group but not in the inactive control group. As participants in the placebo control group were confronted with the chocolate pictures more often than participants in the inactive control group, we expected that the placebo group would show an attenuation of task-induced chocolate craving at posttest as well because of habituation. Finally, we hypothesized that current hunger would be unaffected by the intervention, that is, would be similar across groups and measurements.

(4) Given that short-term effects on food consumption have been reported in approach bias modification studies (ie, reduced consumption after a training session [18]), we expected that chocolate craving and consumption would be reduced on intervention days compared with rest days in the experimental group, and this difference would be larger than in the placebo control group.

In addition to these preregistered analyses, we also explored changes in body mass index and body fat mass as a function of group, examined whether any effects were moderated by baseline levels of trait chocolate craving and restrained eating, and tested whether groups differed in awareness of the study’s aims.


Participants

A power analysis was conducted with G*Power version 3.1.9.2 [22] for repeated measures analysis of variance with a within-between interaction. This revealed that a sample size of 102 (ie, n=34 participants per group) would be sufficient to detect a small effect (f=0.1), given an alpha level of .05, power of .80, 3 groups, 2 measurements, and a correlation of r=.80 between repeated measures.

Participants were recruited at the University of Salzburg and through a local job advertisements website. Inclusion criteria were speaking fluent German, aged between 18 and 50 years, not being pregnant, and not having participated in similar studies in our laboratory. Recruitment advertisements also indicated that participants should be regular chocolate eaters (ie, several times per week) and should not be underweight or currently dieting. A total of 117 individuals responded to the advertisements. A total of 9 participants were excluded before enrollment: 7 participants did not meet inclusion criteria (current pregnancy: n=1, non–German-speaking: n=2, already participated in similar studies in our laboratory: n=4), and 2 participants indicated that they recently decided to refrain from eating chocolate because of lactose intolerance and health reasons (n=2; Figure 1). Of the remaining 108 individuals, 2 did not participate because of technical problems, and 1 discontinued participation (Figure 1). The final sample comprised 105 participants (85.7% female, 90/105) with a mean age of 23.4 years (SD 5.07) and a mean body mass index of 23.3 kg/m² (SD 4.14). The majority of participants had German (52.4%, 55/105) or Austrian (40.0%, 42/105) citizenship and were university students (94.3%, 99/105).

Figure 1. Flow of participants throughout the study. Note that while sample size was n=105 for the majority of analyses, sample size was n=104 for analyses involving body mass index at posttest and n=102 for analyses involving body fat mass at posttest because of missing data.
View this figure

Materials

Approach-Avoidance Task

An AAT was employed to examine whether approach bias toward chocolate-containing foods changed from pre- to posttest as a function of group. The task was programmed in unity (Unity Technologies) and run on a 5-inch SAMSUNG Galaxy J3 smartphone (Samsung Electronics Austria GmbH). A total of 16 pictures of chocolate-containing foods and 16 pictures of nonedible objects were taken from the food pics database [23] (Multimedia Appendix 2). Pictures were matched regarding color, size, brightness, contrast, complexity, recognizability, and familiarity and have previously been used in a joystick-based AAT with which an approach bias toward food was found [24]. The task consisted of 2 blocks: participants were instructed to swipe pictures of food upward (=“away from yourself”) and swipe pictures of objects downward (=“toward yourself”) with the thumb of their dominant hand in 1 block and vice versa in the other block (block order was counterbalanced across participants). Within each block, each picture was presented twice in randomized order. Thus, participants pulled food, pushed food, pulled objects, and pushed objects in 32 trials each, totaling 128 trials. In each trial, 1 picture appeared in the center of the smartphone screen. Similar to joystick-based AATs [25], a zoom effect was employed: picture size increased when the picture was swiped downwards and decreased when the picture was swiped upwards. The picture then disappeared when reaching the border of the screen, and the next trial started (Multimedia Appendix 3).

Sociodemographic and Anthropometric Data

Participants indicated their age, sex, handedness, education, and nationality. Body height (in cm) was measured with a wall-mounted stadiometer. Body weight (in kg) and fat mass (in %) were measured with the OMRON Body Composition Monitor BF511 (OMRON Healthcare Europe BV).

Chocolate Consumption

To examine whether chocolate consumption changed from pre- to posttest as a function of group, participants responded to the question “How often did you consume chocolate-containing foods in the past ten days?” Responses were recorded on a rating slider anchored 0=not at all and 100=very often.

Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-Reduced

The German, chocolate-adapted version of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-reduced (FCQ-T-r) [26] was used to examine whether groups differed at pretest, whether pretest scores moderated any intervention effects, and whether scores changed from pre- to posttest as a function of group. Participants are usually instructed to indicate how frequently each statement is true for them in general. However, to fit the purpose of this study, participants were instructed to indicate how frequently each statement was true for them in the past 10 days. The scale has 15 items (eg, “If I am craving chocolate, thoughts of eating it consume me” and “It is hard for me to resist the temptation to eat chocolate that is in my reach”), which are scored from 1=never to 6=always. Internal reliability was α=.894 at pretest and α=.921 at posttest.

Food Cravings Questionnaire-State

The German, chocolate-adapted version of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-State (FCQ-S) [26] was used to measure current chocolate craving and hunger before and after the AAT. The scale has 15 items (12 items for the chocolate craving subscale and 3 items for the hunger subscale), which are scored from 1=strongly disagree to 5=strongly agree. Internal reliabilities of the chocolate craving subscale ranged between α=.873 and α=.930, and internal reliabilities of the hunger subscale ranged between α=.835 and α=.917 in this study.

Restraint Scale

The German version of the Restraint Scale [27] was used to examine whether groups differed in dietary restraint and whether dietary restraint moderated any intervention effects. The scale has 10 items, which are scored from 0 to 4 (items 1-4 and 10) and 0 to 3 (items 5-9) with different response options. Internal reliability was α=.715 in this study.

Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire

The German version of the Dutch Behavior Questionnaire’s (DEBQ’s) restrained eating subscale [28] was used to examine whether groups differed in dietary restraint and whether dietary restraint moderated any intervention effects. The scale has 10 items, which are scored from 1=never to 5=very often. Internal reliability was α=.879 in this study.

Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire 8

The German version of the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire 8 (EDE-Q8) [29] was used to examine whether groups differed in eating disorder symptomatology. The scale has 8 items that are scored from 0=no days/never/not at all to 6=every day/every time/very much. Internal reliability was α=.883 in this study.

End-of-Day Questions

On each evening during the 10-day period between pre- and posttest, participants answered questions on their smartphone using the application PsyDiary (MultimediaTechnology). Chocolate craving intensity was assessed with the question “How strong was your desire for chocolate-containing foods today (on average)?” Answers were recorded on a rating slider anchored 0=very weak and 100=very strong. Chocolate craving frequency was assessed with the question “How often did you have a desire for chocolate-containing foods today?” Answers were recorded on a rating slider anchored 0=not at all and 100=very often. Chocolate consumption quantity was assessed with the question “How many chocolate-containing foods did you consume today?” Answers were recorded on a rating slider anchored 0=none and 100=a great many. Chocolate consumption frequency was assessed with the question “How often did you consume chocolate-containing foods today?” Answers were recorded on a rating slider anchored 0=not at all and 100=very often.

Debriefing Questions

Awareness of the study’s aims was assessed with the questions “Do you think that the aim of this study was to assess your behavior in relation to chocolate?” and “Do you think that the aim of this study was to change your behavior in relation to chocolate?” Response options for both questions were yes, no, and I don’t know.

Procedure

The study was approved by the ethical review board of the University of Salzburg, and study design and hypotheses were preregistered at aspredicted.org. The study was advertised as a study on “automatic reactions to chocolate-containing foods in daily life.” That is, participants were not informed that the aim of the study was to change chocolate craving and consumption. Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of the 3 groups and were tested in the laboratory individually.

Pretest

At pretest, participants signed informed consent and completed the FCQ-T-r, the question on chocolate consumption in the past 10 days, and the FCQ-S. Afterward, participants practiced the swipe movements in 2 blocks with 10 trials each (which included pictures of animals and household items that were not used in the main task) and then completed the AAT. Then, they completed the FCQ-S again, responded to the sociodemographic questions, and completed the Restraint Scale, the DEBQ, and the EDE-Q8. Subsequently, body height, weight, and fat mass were measured. Finally, participants installed the apps, and the experimenter explained their use, the remaining study procedures, and discussed any open questions. At the end of the day of the pretest, participants received the first prompt (ie, end-of-day questions) to familiarize them with the app (these data were discarded from analyses).

Intervention Period

During the 10-day period between the pre- and posttest, all participants received the end-of-day questions on each evening at 9 pm and could respond to the questions until 10 pm. The experimental group additionally performed 5 training sessions (1 session on 5 days each). Training sessions were similar to the AAT used at pre- and posttest, except that pictures of food were always swiped upwards and pictures of objects were always swiped downwards (ie, there was no reversal of instructions between blocks). The placebo control group also performed 5 training sessions (1 session on 5 days each). Here, training sessions were equal to the AAT used at pre- and posttest, that is, pictures of food and objects were swiped upward or downward equally often. In both the experimental and placebo control group, intervention and rest days were pseudorandomized with a maximum of 3 consecutive intervention or rest days. On intervention days, the training session was available between 12 noon and 8 pm. (reminders were sent every 2 hours). The inactive control group did not perform any training sessions.

Posttest

At posttest, participants again completed the FCQ-T-r, the question on chocolate consumption in the past 10 days, and the FCQ-S; performed the AAT; and then completed the FCQ-S again in the laboratory. Finally, they completed the debriefing questions, and body weight and fat mass were measured. Participation was reimbursed with course credits or €40. The amount of course credits or money was reduced when participants did not complete all signals (ie, training sessions or end-of-day questions).

Data Analyses

Randomization Check and Compliance

We compared groups regarding baseline characteristics with analyses of variance (age, body mass index, body fat mass, chocolate consumption, FCQ-T-r scores, Restraint Scale scores, DEBQ scores, and EDE-Q8 scores) and Fisher exact tests (sex, handedness, education, and nationality). Furthermore, we compared groups regarding the number of completed training sessions (in %) and completed end-of-day questions (in %) with Kruskal-Wallis tests.

Hypothesis 1

Erroneous trials (eg, swipes in the wrong direction) were excluded from analyses. These accounted for 7.27% of all trials at pretest and 10.4% of all trials at posttest. The number of valid trials did not differ between groups (Kruskal-Wallis tests: pretest P=.25, posttest P=.23). Owing to the task setup, we were able to differentiate between 2 different reaction times: the time between picture appearance and participants’ first touch on the screen (touching time) and the time between participants’ first touch on the screen and picture disappearance (dragging time). Bootstrapped split-half reliability estimates for each condition (pull food, push food, pull objects, and push objects) were obtained using the R package splithalf [30] performing 5000 random splits. Reliability estimates for touching time ranged between r=.70 and .77 (Spearman-Brown-corrected rsb=.82-.87) at pretest and between r=.79 and .81 (Spearman-Brown-corrected rsb=.88-.90) at posttest. Reliability estimates for dragging time ranged between r=.69 and .82 (Spearman-Brown-corrected rsb=.82-.90) at pretest and between r=.63 and .83 (Spearman-Brown-corrected rsb=.77-.90) at posttest.

In line with joystick-based AAT studies [25], median reaction times were calculated. As outlined in the preregistration, 3×2×2×2 analyses of variance for repeated measures were calculated with median reaction time data as dependent variables, group (experimental vs placebo control vs inactive control) as between-subjects factor and measurement (pre- vs posttest), stimulus (food vs objects), and direction (pull vs push) as within-subjects factors. This was done separately for touching time and for dragging time (which was not explicitly specified in the preregistration).

Hypothesis 2

As outlined in the preregistration, 3×2 analyses of variance for repeated measures were calculated with self-reported chocolate consumption and FCQ-T-r scores as dependent variables, group (experimental vs placebo control vs inactive control) as between-subjects factor, and measurement (pre- vs posttest) as within-subjects factor.

Hypothesis 3

As outlined in the preregistration, 3×2×2 analyses of variance for repeated measures were calculated with FCQ-S scores (current chocolate craving and hunger) as dependent variables, group (experimental vs placebo control vs inactive control) as between-subjects factor, and measurement (pre- vs posttest) and task (before vs after the task) as within-subjects factors.

Hypothesis 4

Responses to the end-of-day questions on intervention days on which participants did not complete the training session were excluded from analyses. These accounted for 47 signals (6.71%) of the possible 700 signals (10 days×70 participants [experimental + placebo control group]). As outlined in the preregistration, we applied linear mixed models using the R package lme4 [31] to analyze the nested, longitudinal structure of the data. Days (0=rest day, 1=intervention day; Level 1) and group (0=experimental group, 1=placebo control group; Level 2) and their cross-level interaction group × days were used as predictors for chocolate craving intensity/frequency and for chocolate consumption quantity/frequency. We further explored whether pretest scores of the FCQ-T-r, Restraint Scale, and DEBQ at level 2 would modulate any effects. The level 1 predictor days was entered uncentered to the models, and the level 2 predictors group, FCQ-T-r, Restraint Scale, and DEBQ were grand-mean centered. The intercepts of all models were allowed to vary randomly. The data files and R-script for these analyses can be found in Multimedia Appendix 4.

Exploratory Analyses

Analyses of variance for repeated measures with group (experimental vs placebo control vs inactive control) as between-subjects factor and measurement (pre- vs posttest) as within-subjects factor were calculated to examine changes in body mass index and body fat mass as a function of group. Moderation analyses were calculated with PROCESS [32] to examine whether FCQ-T-r scores at pretest, Restraint Scale scores, and DEBQ scores moderated any effects of group on chocolate consumption, body mass index, and body fat mass at posttest while controlling for pretest values. Restraint Scale scores and DEBQ scores were also tested as moderators of effects of group on FCQ-T-r scores at posttest while controlling for FCQ-T-r scores at pretest. Fisher exact tests were calculated to compare groups regarding the 2 debriefing questions. These analyses were not included in the preregistration protocol.


Randomization Check and Compliance

Groups did not differ in any baseline characteristics (Table 1). Compliance was high for both completion of the training sessions (86.6%) and completion of the end-of-day questions (85.8%) and did not differ between groups (Table 1).

Table 1. Means and frequencies of study variables at pretest and compliance rates during the intervention phase as a function of the group (N=105).
Study variablesExperimental group (n=35)Placebo control group (n=35)Inactive control group (n=35)Test statisticsP value
Age (years), mean (SD)22.7 (3.36)24.1 (6.13)23.5 (5.37)F2,102=0.64, ηp²=.012.53
Sex (female), n (%)30 (85.7)32 (91.4)28 (80.0)χ2=1.8, Φ=.133.45
Handedness (right-handed), n (%)28 (80.0)32 (91.4)33 (94.3)χ2=3.5, Φ=.194.23
Education (students), n (%)33 (94.3)32 (91.4)34 (97.1)χ2=1.1, Φ=.101.87
Nationality (German), n (%)16 (45.7)21 (60.0)18 (51.4)χ2=3.5, Φ=.184.47
Body mass index (kg/m²), mean (SD)23.5 (4.90)23.3 (3.62)23.0 (3.89)F2,102=0.16, ηp²=.003.86
Body fat mass (%), mean (SD)31.6 (9.91)32.8 (7.52)29.5 (9.00)F2,102=1.29, ηp²=.025.28
Chocolate consumption (self-report), mean (SD)55.6 (20.9)61.5 (21.3)58.1 (22.8)F2,102=0.65, ηp²=.013.52
Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-reduced (chocolate version), mean (SD)41.4 (8.54)41.2 (10.5)44.2 (12.2)F2,102=0.90, ηp²=.017.41
Restraint Scale, mean (SD)11.5 (5.05)12.0 (4.53)11.7 (4.69)F2,102=0.11, ηp²=.002.89
Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (restrained eating subscale), mean (SD)2.04 (0.80)2.03 (0.60)2.16 (0.65)F2,102=0.38, ηp²=.007.68
Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire 8, mean (SD)0.97 (1.11)1.00 (0.72)1.23 (1.08)F2,102=0.75, ηp²=.014.48
Training sessions compliance (%), mean (SD)89.1 (17.7)84.0 (19.3)a.22
End-of-day questions compliance (%), mean (SD)88.0 (17.5)88.6 (16.1)80.6 (25.6).22

aNot applicable.

Hypothesis 1

Touching Time

A main effect of direction (F1,102=13.3; P<.001; ηp²=.115) indicated that participants touched the target stimuli faster in pull trials (mean 599 ms, SD 55.3) than in push trials (mean 606 ms, SD 56.0). There were significant main effects of measurement and stimulus and interaction effects measurement × stimulus and group × measurement (all Ps<.001), which were qualified by a significant interaction group × measurement × stimulus (F2,102=4.82; P=.01; ηp²=.086). However, as this interaction effect was small, did not include any direction effects, and post-hoc comparisons were inconclusive, it was not further interpreted. More information and a graphical depiction can be found in the supplementary material (Figure S1 in Multimedia Appendix 5). There was no significant main effect of group (F2,102=2.17; P=.12; ηp²=.041) and no other significant interaction effects (all Ps>.16).

Dragging Time

A main effect of stimulus (F1,102=9.46; P=.003; ηp²=.085) indicated that participants swiped food pictures (mean 248 ms, SD 44.5) faster than object pictures (mean 252 ms, SD 53.3). There were no other significant main or interaction effects (all P values>.05).

Hypothesis 2

Chocolate Craving

A main effect of measurement (F1,102=11.7; P=.001; ηp²=.103) indicated that FCQ-T-r scores decreased from pretest (mean 42.3, SD 10.5) to posttest (mean 40.1, SD 11.7). There was no significant main effect of group (F2,102=0.48; P=.62; ηp²=.009) and no significant interaction group × measurement (F2,102=0.79; P=.46; ηp²=.015).

Chocolate Consumption

A main effect of measurement (F1,102=10.3; P=.002; ηp²=.092) indicated that self-reported chocolate consumption decreased from pretest (mean 58.4, SD 21.6) to posttest (mean 51.8, SD 20.2). There was no significant main effect of group (F2,102=0.35; P=.71; ηp²=.007) and no significant interaction group × measurement (F2,102=0.84; P=.44; ηp²=.016).

Hypothesis 3

Current Chocolate Craving

A main effect of task (F1,102=20.7; P<.001; ηp²=.169) indicated that FCQ-S craving scores increased from before (mean 28.0, SD 7.55) to after the task (mean 29.5, SD 8.65). A main effect of measurement (F1,102=17.6; P<.001; ηp²=.147) indicated the FCQ-S craving scores decreased from pretest (mean 30.1, SD 8.01) to posttest (mean 27.4, SD 9.20). There was no significant main effect of group (F2,102=1.06; P=.35; ηp²=.020) and no significant interaction effects (all P values>.46).

Hunger

A main effect of task (F1,102=11.0; P=.001; ηp²=.098) indicated that FCQ-S hunger scores increased from before (mean 7.91, SD 2.74) to after the task (mean 8.20, SD 3.01). There was no significant main effect of measurement (F1,102=1.36; P=.25; ηp²=.013), no significant main effect of group (F2,102=2.46; P=.09; ηp²=.046), and no significant interaction effects (all P values>.43).

Hypothesis 4

Chocolate Craving Intensity and Frequency

There was no significant effect of intervention versus rest days as a function of group (see Table S1 in Multimedia Appendix 5). Higher FCQ-T-r scores at pretest related to higher chocolate craving intensity and frequency, independent of days and group (Table S2). Restrained eating did not relate to chocolate craving intensity or frequency and did not interact with days or group (Table S3, Table S4).

Chocolate Consumption Quantity and Frequency

There was no significant effect of intervention versus rest days as a function of group (Table S5). Higher FCQ-T-r scores at pretest related to higher chocolate consumption quantity and frequency, independent of days and group (Table S6). In addition, a significant days × FCQ-T-r interaction indicated that participants with high trait chocolate craving scores consumed chocolate-containing foods more frequently on intervention than on rest days, irrespective of group (Table S6). Restrained eating did not relate to chocolate craving quantity or frequency and did not interact with days or group (Table S7, Table S8).

Exploratory Analyses

Body Mass Index

There were no significant main effects and no interaction effect group × measurement (all P values>.56).

Body Fat Mass

A main effect of measurement (F1,99=4.43; P=.04; ηp²=.043) indicated that body fat mass decreased from pretest (mean 31.4, SD 8.49) to posttest (mean 31.1, SD 8.73). There was no significant main effect of group (F2,99=0.80; P=.45; ηp²=.016) and no significant interaction group × measurement (F2,99=0.30; P=.74; ηp²=.006).

Moderation Analyses

There were no significant interaction effects between group and FCQ-T-r, Restraint Scale, and DEBQ scores at pretest (all P values>.24).

Debriefing Questions

A total of 93 participants (88.6%, 93/105) indicated that they thought the aim of the study was to assess their behavior in relation to chocolate, 4 participants (3.8%, 4/105) did not think so, and 8 participants (7.6%, 8/105) indicated that they did not know. There were no significant differences between groups (χ2=4.6; P=.30; Φ=.224). A total of 29 participants (27.6%, 29/105) indicated that they thought the aim of the study was to change their behavior in relation to chocolate, 61 participants (58.1%, 61/105) did not think so, and 15 participants (14.3%, 15/105) indicated that they did not know. Here, responses did significantly differ between groups (χ2=9.63; P=.04; Φ=.317): more participants in the inactive control group (n=26) did not think that the study’s aim was to change their behavior than participants in both the experimental group (n=18) and the placebo control group (n=17), whereas the latter 2 groups did not differ from each other (based on follow-up z tests using α=.05).


Summary of Results

This study examined effects of a smartphone-based approach-avoidance intervention on approach bias toward chocolate-containing foods and chocolate craving/consumption relative to placebo and no training conditions. The 3 groups were well matched at baseline, treatment adherence was high (87% completed training sessions), and study attrition was low. All dependent measures evidenced good-to-excellent reliability. However, a smartphone-based AAT did neither reveal an approach bias toward chocolate-containing foods at baseline nor a modulation through training. In fact, chocolate craving and consumption decreased throughout the study period in all 3 groups. This self-report finding was corroborated in that participants in all groups lost body fat. Crucially, only a minority of participants thought that this study’s aim was to change their behavior, suggesting that these effects were not because of demand characteristics. Comparing chocolate craving and consumption on intervention versus rest days did not reveal any short-term effects of the training.

Measuring and Modifying Approach-Avoidance Tendencies With Swipe Movements

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that aimed at measuring and changing an approach bias toward food stimuli based on swipe movements on smartphones. Although there are similar studies that examined effects of smartphone-based approach-avoidance training with swipe movements on procrastination and body dissatisfaction [12,13], these studies did not measure effects of the training on approach-avoidance tendencies. Thus, the lack of finding and modifying an approach bias toward chocolate-containing foods may be related to an insensitivity of our newly developed task to detect such effects. However, several arguments speak against such an interpretation. First, we used the same stimuli with which an approach bias toward food was detected in a comparable sample with a joystick-based AAT [24]. Second, the AAT in this study had moderate-to-good internal reliability [33] and, thus, unreliability of the task is unlikely to account for the current lack of findings. Third, increasing evidence indicates that the type of arm movements (flexion and extension; [34]) or distance change [35] is not essential for measuring or modifying approach-avoidance inclinations. For example, it has been found that upward and downward movements or framing actions as approach and avoidance suffice to modify stimulus evaluations [36]. Nevertheless, future research needs to determine whether other techniques such as moving the smartphone toward and away with arm movements [37,38] or using tilt movements [39] are better suited for detecting and changing approach-avoidance tendencies with smartphones. In addition, it has recently been found that combining approach-avoidance actions with affective feedback produced stronger changes in food choices than conventional approach-avoidance training [40]. Thus, using such consequence-based approach-avoidance training may similarly enhance training effects with smartphone-based implementations.

Effects of Monitoring Food Intake

Another consideration is that—even if the approach-avoidance training had an effect—it may have been masked by the general decreases in outcome variables across the study period that were observed regardless of group assignment. Specifically, we included daily end-of-day-questions in the study design to be able to examine short-term effects (ie, on the same day) of the single training sessions. However, these questions may have acted as a type of ecological momentary intervention [41]. For example, it has been shown that keeping a daily snack diary reduced snacking frequency, suggesting that cue monitoring suffices to decrease unhealthy food intake (irrespective of additional intervention modules; [42]), potentially through increased awareness for one’s eating behavior. In fact, it has been found that self-monitoring in terms of completing a record of snacking once per day in the evening decreased snack food consumption even in samples that are not particularly motivated to change their behavior [19]. Thus, we cannot fully exclude the possibility that the intervention may have effects—albeit small—on eating behavior that were masked by effects of monitoring food intake.

Limitations

Interpretation of results needs to consider the sample investigated in this study. Although we included both men and women with a body mass index ranging from underweight to obese, the majority of the sample were normal-weight women. It has been previously suggested that successful retraining of appetitive reactions and consumption behaviors may primarily be found in clinical samples [9]. Although we investigated a nonclinical sample, it is worth noting that our participants had above-average mean scores (>40; Table 1) on the FCQ-T-r (mean scores were 35 in study 1 and 34 in study 2 in the validation studies; [26]), and their eating behavior was clearly impacted throughout the study period (ie, measures were sensitive to detect training-induced changes). This renders insufficient levels of trait chocolate craving as an explanation for these findings unlikely.

Several other methodological considerations might account for these results. For example, although we selected food stimuli with which we have previously detected an approach bias in a comparable sample using a joystick-based task [24], it may be that approach-avoidance training work better when using personalized stimuli, that is, pictures of foods that participants actually crave and consume regularly in their daily life. In related research on attentional bias, for example, it has been found that internal reliability of reaction time tasks can be increased when personalized stimuli are used [43]. Furthermore, we used relatively few training sessions (5), which may have been insufficient to produce meaningful changes in approach bias and eating behavior. However, evidence from joystick-based approach-avoidance training suggest that few sessions suffice to detect such effects in relation to alcohol [44]. Yet, other smartphone-based studies did indeed use more frequent training sessions [12,13]. Thus, the number of training sessions required in smartphone-based approach-avoidance training need further examination. Finally, although we instructed participants regarding the meaning of upward and downward swipe movements, we did not assess whether they actually perceived the movements as pushing or pulling the pictures away from or toward themselves. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that participants did not perceive the movements as intended, which could explain the lack of finding an approach bias and training effects.

Conclusions

Repeatedly avoiding chocolate-containing foods in terms of (zoom out) upward swipe movements on smartphones did not change behavior related to these foods in this study. Owing to several methodological considerations, there is an urgent need for future research that determines the most effective way of measuring and changing approach-avoidance tendencies in daily life. General decreases in chocolate craving and consumption as well as body fat mass in this study may be because of the generally raised awareness of chocolate consumption throughout the study period. Thus, receiving daily prompts for monitoring food intake may be a cheap and efficient way to normalize food intake in individuals with eating disorders and facilitate weight loss in individuals with obesity.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Thérèse Hamm, Maike Burkholder, Lina Lahmer, Lisa-Lucia Ernst, and Veronika Kainz for collecting the data.

This study was supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (ERC-StG-2014 639445 NewEat). The funder had no involvement in designing this study, data collection, analysis and interpretation of data, writing of this report, or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Preregistration.

PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 131 KB

Multimedia Appendix 2

Stimuli.

RAR File , 2916 KB

Multimedia Appendix 3

Example trials video.

MP4 File (MP4 Video), 2593 KB

Multimedia Appendix 4

Study data.

RAR File , 36 KB

Multimedia Appendix 5

Supplementary material.

PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 912 KB

Multimedia Appendix 6

CONSORT-EHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.1).

PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 98 KB

  1. Wiers RW, Rinck M, Kordts R, Houben K, Strack F. Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction 2010 Feb;105(2):279-287. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  2. Eberl C, Wiers RW, Pawelczack S, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Dev Cogn Neurosci 2013 Apr;4:38-51 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Wiers RW, Eberl C, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients' approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychol Sci 2011 Apr;22(4):490-497. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Brockmeyer T, Hahn C, Reetz C, Schmidt U, Friederich H. Approach bias modification in food craving-a proof-of-concept study. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2015 Sep;23(5):352-360. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Mehl N, Mueller-Wieland L, Mathar D, Horstmann A. Retraining automatic action tendencies in obesity. Physiol Behav 2018 Aug 1;192:50-58. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Warschburger P, Gmeiner M, Morawietz M, Rinck M. Battle of plates: a pilot study of an approach-avoidance training for overweight children and adolescents. Public Health Nutr 2018 Feb;21(2):426-434. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Ferentzi H, Scheibner H, Wiers R, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J, Beisel S, et al. Retraining of automatic action tendencies in individuals with obesity: a randomized controlled trial. Appetite 2018 Jul 1;126:66-72. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Leeman RF, Nogueira C, Wiers RW, Cousijn J, Serafini K, DeMartini KS, et al. A test of multisession automatic action tendency retraining to reduce alcohol consumption among young adults in the context of a human laboratory paradigm. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2018 Apr;42(4):803-814 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Krishna A, Eder AB. No effects of explicit approach-avoidance training on immediate consumption of soft drinks. Appetite 2018 Nov 1;130:209-218. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Becker D, Jostmann NB, Holland RW. Does approach bias modification really work in the eating domain? A commentary on Kakoschke et al (2017). Addict Behav 2018 Feb;77:293-294. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Kakoschke N, Kemps E, Tiggemann M. Approach bias modification training and consumption: a review of the literature. Addict Behav 2017 Jan;64:21-28. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Kollei I, Lukas CA, Loeber S, Berking M. An app-based blended intervention to reduce body dissatisfaction: a randomized controlled pilot study. J Consult Clin Psychol 2017 Nov;85(11):1104-1108. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Lukas CA, Berking M. Reducing procrastination using a smartphone-based treatment program: a randomized controlled pilot study. Internet Interv 2018 Jun;12:83-90 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Weingarten HP, Elston D. Food cravings in a college population. Appetite 1991 Dec;17(3):167-175. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Richard A, Meule A, Reichenberger J, Blechert J. Food cravings in everyday life: an EMA study on snack-related thoughts, cravings, and consumption. Appetite 2017 Jun 1;113:215-223. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Becker D, Jostmann NB, Wiers RW, Holland RW. Approach avoidance training in the eating domain: testing the effectiveness across three single session studies. Appetite 2015 Feb;85:58-65. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Dickson H, Kavanagh DJ, MacLeod C. The pulling power of chocolate: effects of approach-avoidance training on approach bias and consumption. Appetite 2016 Apr 1;99:46-51. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Schumacher SE, Kemps E, Tiggemann M. Bias modification training can alter approach bias and chocolate consumption. Appetite 2016 Jan 1;96:219-224. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Maas J, Hietbrink L, Rinck M, Keijsers GP. Changing automatic behavior through self-monitoring: does overt change also imply implicit change? J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2013 Sep;44(3):279-284. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Meule A, Lutz AP, Krawietz V, Stützer J, Vögele C, Kübler A. Food-cue affected motor response inhibition and self-reported dieting success: a pictorial affective shifting task. Front Psychol 2014;5:216 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Meule A, Skirde AK, Freund R, Vögele C, Kübler A. High-calorie food-cues impair working memory performance in high and low food cravers. Appetite 2012 Oct;59(2):264-269. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007 May;39(2):175-191. [CrossRef]
  23. Blechert J, Meule A, Busch NA, Ohla K. Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front Psychol 2014;5:617 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Lender A, Meule A, Rinck M, Brockmeyer T, Blechert J. Measurement of food-related approach-avoidance biases: larger biases when food stimuli are task relevant. Appetite 2018 Jun 1;125:42-47. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Rinck M, Becker ES. Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2007 Jun;38(2):105-120. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Meule A, Hormes JM. Chocolate versions of the Food Cravings Questionnaires. Associations with chocolate exposure-induced salivary flow and ad libitum chocolate consumption. Appetite 2015 Aug;91:256-265. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Dinkel A, Berth H, Exner C, Rief W, Balck F. German version of the Restraint Scale for the assessment of restrained eating. Diagnostica 2005 Apr;51(2):67-74. [CrossRef]
  28. Grunert S. Ein Inventar zur Erfassung von Selbstaussagen zum Ernährungsverhalten. Diagnostica 1989;35:167-179.
  29. Kliem S, Mößle T, Zenger M, Strauß B, Brähler E, Hilbert A. The eating disorder examination-questionnaire 8: a brief measure of eating disorder psychopathology (EDE-Q8). Int J Eat Disord 2016 Jun;49(6):613-616. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Parsons S. figshare. 2018. Splithalf: robust estimates of split half reliability (Version 4).   URL: https://figshare.com/articles/splithalf/5559175 [accessed 2019-09-25]
  31. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Soft 2015;67(1):1-48. [CrossRef]
  32. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. Second Edition. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2018.
  33. Parsons S, Kruijt A, Fox E. Psychological science needs a standard practice of reporting the reliability of cognitive behavioural measurements. PsyArXiv 2018. [CrossRef]
  34. Krieglmeyer R, de Houwer J, Deutsch R. On the nature of automatically triggered approach–avoidance behavior. Emotion Rev 2013 Jun 11;5(3):280-284. [CrossRef]
  35. Eder AB, Rothermund K. When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen 2008 May;137(2):262-281. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. van Dessel P, Eder AB, Hughes S. Mechanisms underlying effects of approach-avoidance training on stimulus evaluation. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2018 Aug;44(8):1224-1241. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Zech HG. ResearchGate. 2015. The Mobile Approach-Avoidance Task.   URL: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/282943650_The_mobile_approach-avoidance_task [accessed 2019-09-25]
  38. Cring C. Leiden Repository. 2017. Approach and Avoidance Tendencies: Using Smartphones to Study Behavioral Responses and Food Desires.   URL: https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/53247 [accessed 2019-09-25]
  39. Kakoschke N, Hawker C, Castine B, de Courten B, Verdejo-Garcia A. Smartphone-based cognitive bias modification training improves healthy food choice in obesity: a pilot study. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2018 Sep;26(5):526-532. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. van Dessel P, Hughes S, de Houwer J. Consequence-based approach-avoidance training: a new and improved method for changing behavior. Psychol Sci 2018 Dec;29(12):1899-1910. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Heron KE, Smyth JM. Ecological momentary interventions: incorporating mobile technology into psychosocial and health behaviour treatments. Br J Health Psychol 2010 Feb;15(Pt 1):1-39 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Verhoeven AA, Adriaanse MA, de Vet E, Fennis BM, de Ridder DT. Identifying the 'if' for 'if-then' plans: combining implementation intentions with cue-monitoring targeting unhealthy snacking behaviour. Psychol Health 2014;29(12):1476-1492. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Christiansen P, Mansfield R, Duckworth J, Field M, Jones A. Internal reliability of the alcohol-related visual probe task is increased by utilising personalised stimuli and eye-tracking. Drug Alcohol Depend 2015 Oct 1;155:170-174. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Eberl C, Wiers RW, Pawelczack S, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Implementation of approach bias re-training in alcoholism-how many sessions are needed? Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2014 Feb;38(2):587-594. [CrossRef] [Medline]


AAT: approach-avoidance task
DEBQ: Dutch Behavior Questionnaire
EDE-Q8: Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire 8
EMA: ecological momentary assessment
FCQ-S: Food Cravings Questionnaire-State
FCQ-T-r: Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-reduced


Edited by G Eysenbach; submitted 24.09.18; peer-reviewed by R Leeman, D Becker; comments to author 01.11.18; revised version received 03.02.19; accepted 24.03.19; published 01.11.19

Copyright

©Adrian Meule, Anna Richard, Radomir Dinic, Jens Blechert. Originally published in JMIR Mhealth and Uhealth (http://mhealth.jmir.org), 01.11.2019.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR mhealth and uhealth, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://mhealth.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.